“8” in “Curiosity Studies”
8
Autism, Neurodiversity, and Curiosity
Kristina T. Johnson
What we observe is not nature itself, but nature exposed to our method of questioning.
—Werner Heisenberg
One morning in early April, on a beautiful Boston day, I watched as a little boy stopped next to an old brick wall outside the local dry cleaner. Most of the bricks were a rusty red, but a few were a variegated black, and one black brick seemed to capture his attention in particular. He tapped it and ran his finger across it, tilting his head and swaying, seeming to contemplate it for a few minutes before resuming his walk to the park.
What about that brick had made him stop? Was he curious about why the brick was black among all its red neighbors? Had he tapped it to determine if black bricks felt different than other bricks, or had he just been randomly stroking bricks and happened to stop at that one? Maybe it was purely visual: he was intrigued by the high contrast of the black on red and paused a moment to enjoy the view. Or perhaps it was nothing at all; he had just stopped to take a break.
I shared several of these thoughts with the boy while we stood by the wall, pointing out my observations and remarking on the wall’s texture and composition. But he gave no observable indication that he had heard me, understood me, or cared what I had said. He did not look at me, gesture for my attention, or share his thought process. I was but an outside observer to his rich inner world.
The little boy, Gabriel, has autism, and he happens to be my son. He also has a rare genetic disorder that inhibits motor planning, including the most sophisticated motor planning that humans perform—speech. While it is not uncommon for autistic individuals to have speech and communication challenges, individuals with this genetic disorder never master the motor planning necessary to speak even single words, and, to date, Gabriel is the first person missing the main gene of this disorder to walk independently. He has profound cognitive delays—the gene he’s missing usually helps strengthen the connections between brain cells when something is learned—and what is effortless for most infants is a Herculean feat for him.
Still, does the fact that he can never tell me that he is curious or move in ways typically associated with curiosity imply that he is not curious? Of course not. But scientifically, how can we measure his curiosity? How can we characterize it, probe it, or understand its dimensions and dynamics in the ways that science offers neurotypical individuals?1 Understanding Gabriel’s curiosity—and that of every other child who deviates from the norm—requires both asking new questions and finding new ways of asking questions so that we share in the exceptional intricacy and diversity of their worlds.
Neurodiverse Curiosity Studies
For centuries scientists have been captivated by curiosity, the motivated exploratory behavior that seems to underlie human development. Nineteenth-century philosopher and psychologist William James named curiosity immediately following fear and love in his list of native reactions comprising the “impulses and instincts of childhood.”2 In the early twentieth century, Jean Piaget observed infants exploring objects from the earliest age, discovering that their actions produced effects.3 In the 1950s and ’60s, Daniel Berlyne sparked a new era of inquiry into the causation of curiosity, positing a neurophysiological mechanism for this behavior. He suggested that uncertainty in the environment induced a state of arousal, prompting exploration to reduce this uncertainty.4 Still others have theorized that curiosity originates from an information gap between what one knows and what one wants to know,5 an incongruity between expected and unexpected outcomes,6 or simply an innate drive, propelling organisms to explore and work with no obvious reward.7 And while no single unified definition or theory of curiosity has emerged,8 almost every approach to the study of curiosity has had a common thread: they have all focused on typical development. Individuals who diverged strongly from the mean have been given their own studies, but usually only to identify the ways in which they differed from the “typical” population.9
For example, in one of the earliest studies of curiosity and autism,10 researchers found that autistic individuals (N = 5, ages 4–7) took four times longer, on average, to approach a foot-wide red metal box with a lever on top than a group of typically developing 3–5 year olds.11 The researchers also found that the autistic children spent less time exploring the box once they approached it, and that they quickly became uninterested in the box and its lever—much sooner than the “typically developing” group of participants. Autistic children exhibited diminished interest in the red box and, in doing so, their exploratory activity was deemed deficient.
Yet, in the middle of the paper, the authors mention that these children showed more than a hundredfold increase in lever interaction in the two directions that produced sounds, triggering a bell or a buzzer, suggesting that they were significantly more motivated by the sounds than anything else about the box. Since most of the autistic children had been initially referred to the doctor for “probable deafness,” a common referral in young autistic individuals because of their lack of response to language,12 the authors note this finding as “interesting” but give it little further discussion.
Almost fifty years later, we recognize these findings as more than interesting; they are indicative of the need to reexamine the ways in which we study individuals with autism and other developmental differences. Even accounting for the small sample size, the participants were clearly attracted to the sounds and sought more input from the bell and the buzzer than anything else about the experimental setup. While this does indeed differ from canonical exploratory behavior in neurotypical children, the lack of exploration is not the conclusion. Rather, their novel and intense exploration of unique phenomena forms the basis for a new line of inquiry.
What else would these individuals have explored if there had been a greater number and variety of auditory stimuli? Would chords or short ditties have had the same effect? Since many autistic children, including the children in the study, show decreased attention to human voice, how does their exploratory behavior change when the response is a spoken word versus a sung one? Or songs with lyrics compared to instrumental pieces?
These more nuanced lines of inquiry can suggest crucial study variables, allowing a systematic examination of the effects of certain stimuli for each child. By first establishing the way in which an individual excels in exploring, we can construct a baseline of activity and behavior for that child founded upon their strengths. We can characterize the intensity, duration, and diversity of their exploration and curiosity, as well as the features of the information or stimuli. We can also measure peripheral responses, such as their affect, interactions, and physiology. Then we can empirically investigate the parameters that alter the individual’s exploration.
For example, how does the type of stimuli or the presentation of the stimuli alter the exploration? Berlyne used visually intricate patterns to determine the optimal complexity that engaged one’s attention for the greatest amount of time in order to map the properties of curiosity stimuli.13 However, studies have indicated that autistic individuals tend to explore images that they find personally interesting for longer and in a more detail-oriented manner than other images.14 How, then, might visual attention be altered when the stimulus is not a pattern but rather an increasingly complex image of a child’s favorite character or topic? If attention, as measured by gaze persistence, is plotted as a function of complexity, does it change over time for that child? Do variations in auditory complexity—for example, slight alterations of a favorite song—follow a similar curve to their visual counterparts?
While there is no shortage of parameters and variables one can explore, the intent is not to make the system endlessly more complex. Rather, our goal is to isolate the key variables that characterize an individual’s natural curiosity. Once established, we can form and test hypotheses to probe this curiosity and compare data across individuals with similar characterizations. These individuals may not be grouped by traditional divisions, such as age or diagnoses, but they may display similar manifestations of curiosity. Then, within individuals or within groups, we can systematically investigate how small changes of certain parameters alter the manifestation of curiosity. Ultimately, this methodological, individualized process can produce a framework to inform the field of neurodiverse curiosity studies.
A Note on Language
While the CDC recommends using “person-first” language when referring to individuals with various diagnoses or distinguishing traits (e.g., “person with autism,” “individual with a disability”) in an effort to ensure that the individual is not defined by the diagnosis, label, or trait, many persons and groups prefer “identity-first” language, such as “autistic person” or “disabled individual.”15 Similar to many members of the Deaf community, these individuals feel that the features that motivated the diagnosis or labels are inherently tied to their identities.16 Recognizing that identity is deeply personal, both expressions will be utilized interchangeably throughout this work.
In addition, this paper will attempt to address both autism and intellectual disability (ID), understanding that each of these terms describes a spectrum and that they are not mutually inclusive. Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) can and do exist without accompanying ID, and ID can be completely detached from autistic behaviors or ASD diagnoses. However, both groups have been marginalized within studies of curiosity, and the research methods that augment the studies of one may also bolster studies of the other. And in many of the severest forms of either label, these diagnoses exist together, and, thus, one must account for the superposition of these spectra as well.
Finally, I acknowledge that all of the words and phrases employed here today are subject to the “euphemism treadmill,” whereby words slowly evolve in meaning to become offensive, even if they were originally introduced to replace an offensive word.17 Nevertheless, the intention of this work is accessibility in the broadest sense, and I look forward to improved inclusive lexical semantics in future work.
Case Studies
To further elucidate this inclusive framework, I present three individual profiles. (Names have been changed.) These case studies have been chosen not as a complete, representative sample but rather as a foundation upon which other neurodiverse studies of curiosity can build. The profiles were acquired through a combination of parent report and personal observation and focus primarily on preadolescent children; however, a broad range of language abilities and developmental stages have been included with the hope that many of the principles presented here can be extrapolated to other persons and populations.
Within each profile I share an overview of the individual’s development to provide context and highlight the breadth of neurodiversity, especially across the autism spectrum. More important, I note ways that we could build the scientific method around their strengths, often employing technology to motivate new ways of asking questions and understanding the answers.
Mark, Age Ten
Mark is a ten-and-a-half-year-old boy with a diagnosis of ASD. He also has a learning disability and has been referred for concerns regarding attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but his parents have not sought a diagnosis for the latter.
At two years of age, Mark had only a few functional spoken words. By age four, his language had dramatically improved to include sentences. He could engage in two-way conversations for a few minutes when motivated; however, his day-to-day use of language was still consistent with the typical early to mid-two-year-old level. At ten and a half, he converses at a similar rate and length to his peers, though his topics tend to focus heavily on things that interest him, such as dinosaurs and dragons.
His parents describe him as “very curious,” “analytical,” and “metacognitive”; he is always trying to ascertain how he thinks and operates. He loves to ask questions, including “why” questions, a hallmark of traditional curiosity questions.18 Mark also enjoys experimenting with material properties and the effects of additives like hot water and soap. For example, he will take toy dinosaurs made of different types of material, including foam, rubber, and hard plastic, into the bathroom and will systematically add different amounts of water (e.g., little or lots) at different temperatures (e.g., hot, warm, or cold) with various additives (e.g., soap or shampoo). These experiments are self-driven and often performed surreptitiously, under the perception that his parents are unaware of his actions.
Although it may seem like Mark would perform admirably in typical studies of curiosity, asking him to play in this way or prompting him with other materials or activities does not necessarily result in similar exploration. In a classic study by Kreitler, Zigler, and Kreitler,19 children were presented with familiar everyday objects, such as “medium-sized toys of a car, an iron, a telephone, and a piano.” The researchers then counted the number of responses the child used to describe the items, as well as the number of “inspective” manipulations (i.e., visually inspecting the toy), “customary” manipulations (e.g., playing the piano, answering the pretend phone), and “exploratory” manipulations (e.g., trying to take the toy apart or determine how it operates). These variables were combined with others to discern different types of curiosity.
Indeed, Mark has rarely shown interest in typical everyday objects. In fact, Mark’s parents reported not having to childproof the house in the way that most parents must because he never approached the knobs on the stove or other items that typically draw children’s attention. Even though Kreitler and colleagues were one of the first groups to acknowledge that curiosity is “neither unitary nor homogenous” and encouraged evaluating a wide range of curiosity types before characterizing a child as “curious or noncurious,” their proposed curiosity measures based on typical development would almost certainly capture Mark’s weaknesses but not his strengths. He would likely be labeled “not curious.”
In what ways could we capture his strengths? How can we characterize his curiosity in a way that is both personalized and rigorous? Like many autistic individuals, Mark exhibits strong specific interests in certain topics or objects that persist longer and with greater intensity than those of nonautistic individuals.20 For example, for the last five years, Mark has been particularly enthralled by dinosaurs and dragons. Mark’s self-stated goal in life is to become the “first hybrid human-dinosaur,” and he independently uses the internet and other resources to learn about anatomy and genetics with the goal of altering his DNA to give himself wings to fly like a dragon.
Figure 8.1. SPRING, a customizable learning platform.
These devout interests, or “affinities,” can be leveraged to motivate or evoke exploration and inquiry in ways that might otherwise remain hidden,21 as in the case of the bathroom dinosaur science experiments. Affinities also provide a unique opportunity to investigate how different reinforcement mechanisms alter a child’s exploratory behavior. For example, we have developed a research platform called SPRING that enables customization in the activity, the development level of the activity, and the reinforcement provided by the system (Figure 8.1).22 It is similar in style to Banta’s curiosity box, but wholly customizable and programmable.23 The central module, shown as shape sorters in Figure 8.1, can be removed and replaced with different activities to match the developmental skills and interests of the child. Sensors within SPRING passively record user activity, minimizing the effects of examiner presence or intervention while enabling quantitative objective data collection.
By modifying the parameters of SPRING, one can systematically examine a child’s exploration as a function of various reinforcement parameters or levels of ambiguity. For instance, since Mark loves dragons, SPRING could be programmed such that every time Mark inserts a square followed by two circles, his favorite dragon video appears on the device. Inserting other shapes might produce sounds and lights, but no dragons. Thus the significance of dragons as a variable in his exploratory behavior might be examined. By varying the reinforcement, including no reinforcement, the intrinsic and extrinsic motivational factors behind his exploration and problem solving could also be appraised.24 Similarly, prompts could be shown on the SPRING screen, as displayed in Figure 8.1, or the screen could be left blank to examine his exploration and problem solving without assistance. Since SPRING is highly customizable, it and similar personalizable devices can unlock a whole domain of individualized data-driven curiosity studies.
Gabriel, Age Seven
As discussed earlier, Gabriel struggles with motor planning, language, and general learning. While expressive language is expected to be a lifelong struggle, receptive language has also been markedly challenging. Gabriel did not respond in any discernible way to spoken language until shortly before his fifth birthday, and then it was only to the word “car,” referencing the family car, where a few of his favorite toys were kept. Between ages six and seven, he showed considerable growth, with occasional clear recognition of words like “dinner,” “cow” (for a favorite dancing cow toy), and “watch” (as in, “watch a movie”); however, other words, including his name, continue to produce no overt response.
Undoubtedly, attempts to use language to discern the mechanics and motivations of Gabriel’s mind will fall short. Even with alternative means of communication like an augmentative assistive communication (AAC) device, sign language, gestures, picture cards, and other cues, his vocabulary is limited to a few dozen words. And while communication is a major hurdle, it is not the only one. Standardized assessments consistently rate him either in the most severe categories, or such assessments cannot be used because he cannot complete them in the way they were intended. He has been recruited for over a dozen scientific studies, usually ones advertised specifically for autism, but he has not met the inclusion criteria for any of them. His lack of language, cognitive delays, and motor challenges cement his position outside the community of scientific inquiry.
Yet it is not Gabriel’s responsibility to evolve to fit the mold of science. It is our duty as scientists to develop new ways to understand Gabriel. We need a new research framework that captures meaningful data on how he acts and, maybe someday, on how he thinks. We must ask new questions while jointly creating new ways to answer those questions. For example, how does movement of the stimulus vary the perceived complexity of the stimulus? What is the role of agency in exploration? How does personal interest in the topic or object alter curiosity behaviors?
Gabriel explores his environment primarily through sensorimotor actions, including stroking materials to explore textures, spinning items to explore visual dynamics, or tapping objects to surfaces to explore sounds. He finds visually engaging experiences, such as shadows, waterfalls, and dynamic light displays like scrolling LED tickers captivating. Like many autistic individuals, he seems to possess heightened sensory awareness. Some find this hypersensitivity aversive, but Gabriel seeks it out, suggesting a high threshold for optimal sensory input.
But while his sensory seeking is pervasive, it is also specific. A few years ago, a wide balance beam was built for Gabriel that lit up directly in front of his foot as he stepped on it, designed to provide a more enticing environment for him to practice stabilizing and walking. The colored lights were originally programmed to flash three times with each step, and, although Gabriel generally loves lights, he showed little interest. However, when the lights were changed to chase one another down the LED strip, similar to marquee lights, he eagerly approached the beam, willingly practicing challenging skills in order to activate the lights. Such an experience is reminiscent of the curiosity study with the autistic children manipulating the lever, except here we find that not only does the type of stimuli matter (e.g., lights, sounds, and textures) but the dynamics (e.g., how it moves, how it changes) affect the course of action as well. Therefore, within the realm of neurodiverse curiosity studies, and perhaps within all curiosity studies, we must consider what novel features, such as the dynamics of the stimuli, specifically characterize an individual’s curiosity.
Moreover, it is not only what features or variables characterize a person’s curiosity, but also how that curiosity evolves. Recently, Gabriel discovered that a pillow covered in sequins reflects sunlight from a window, covering the walls in golden sparkles of light (Figure 8.2). Twisting the pillow altered the design, intensity, and location of sparkles on the wall. Upon discovery, he spent more than thirty minutes exploring the various patterns of light, his rapt attention far exceeding his usual concentration on a single task or activity. He returned to the pillow and the window for further exploration on subsequent days. Eventually, he began playing with cars and stuffed animals—toys that had never independently held his interest—on top of the pillow (and on top of the pillow only), seemingly motivated to continue exploring the reflection of light in multiple constructs. His curiosity had a distinctive temporal evolution, growing and changing with time.
Even so, developmental assessments would suggest that this exploration is simply a manifestation of his overt developmental age. But is it infantile, or is it artistic? If Gabriel were able to describe his experience in a way that could be understood and shared, we would almost certainly ascribe it to the latter. With language, Gabriel might share the beauty he sees when the reflections cascade across the wall or wonder aloud why sunlight creates the patterns of light while the living room light does not. Without language, it is easy to assume that his actions are merely basal sensory exploration. How could we know differently?
We need new metrics. Berlyne clearly delineates between “perceptual” (stimulus-based) and “epistemic” (knowledge-based) curiosity.25 The former is the major driver in the exploration of novel stimuli, one shared by animals, infants, and adults alike. In that case, however, the physiological arousal induced by the novelty of the stimuli is expected to decrease after repeated exposure, which, in turn, reduces interest in the stimuli.26 Yet Gabriel exhibited no such arousal reduction nor a diminishing interest over time. His interest appeared to grow with time, naturally scaffolding and expanding his exploration to different “sparkle” environments and interactions with other toys, suggesting something distinctive from the classical definitions of both perceptual and epistemic curiosity.
Figure 8.2. Gabriel manipulates his sparkle pillow to reflect light.
While researchers acknowledge that many variants of curiosity have yet to be fully defined, they still rely on measures like questionnaires and prescribed laboratory studies to test their theories.27 But Gabriel cannot yet explain the motivations for his actions with the sparkle pillow using words, nor can he share his potentially profound sensory experiences in a way that can be cataloged or measured. No clinical study of curiosity would capture, or even replicate, this experience, as it appeared to occur almost through happenstance. How can we test these theories of curiosity for neurodiverse children?
Wearable physiological sensors—for example, watches, glasses, headbands, badges, and clothing that can detect or infer the wearer’s physiological signals without involving wires and constrained environments—may enable a modern approach to tackling these questions and theories. These sensors can track the body’s sympathetic nervous system arousal through electrodermal activity (EDA; formerly galvanic skin response).28 They can also monitor heart rate, heart rate variability, skin temperature, posture, and respiration rate.29 In fact, emerging systems do not even require the use of a wearable device; they can passively survey a person’s physiology through standard cameras, combining light and color magnification with machine-learning analysis techniques.30 With these tools, we can capture and characterize Gabriel’s arousal systematically, and we can do so in a naturalistic environment. We can plot his arousal as a function of activity and behavior and monitor it over time. Is his physiological arousal a function of his curiosity, or is his curiosity a function of his arousal? Does his EDA evolve as his play extends beyond sparkles on the wall to include the reflections on trucks and books that he holds above the pillow? If spikes in arousal initially precede his exploratory behavior, how does this relationship change as his play expands?
Gabriel also has a penchant for music. Although his motor delays inhibit his ability to create sophisticated sounds without assistance, he shows heightened awareness and sensitivity to music over other auditory input. Even as a young toddler, he would choose, through the use of picture cards, to watch video recordings of piano concertos and symphonies, including complex compositions by Rachmaninoff and Dvorak, over animated movie clips or other “age-appropriate” offerings. Years later, he freely and extensively explores the different demo tracks on a digital keyboard, using the programming buttons to modify the tempo, rhythm, and dynamics. But the keyboard is complex. It took years of exposure for him to manually operate the multistep tempo and track buttons, and he still lacks agency to deliberately and accurately select demo tracks. He clearly prefers certain tracks, however, because he will guide a friend or family member over to the keyboard and will continue requesting new music until landing on his favorite song. His lack of agency undeniably impedes his expression of musical curiosity.
Would a control panel with simpler, one-step buttons enable a more extensive, and perhaps more genuine, manifestation of his curiosity? Such a panel would have the added bonus of being able to track button presses and other actions, providing an objective log of his musical exploration over time. Could the differing exploration between the two systems—the complex keyboard and the simpler control panel—measure curiosity as a function of agency? Multiple panels could be built with relative ease, each one increasing slightly in complexity or capabilities by offering more songs or features. Does Gabriel always prefer the simplest access to music, suggesting that it is more about sensory feedback than exploration, or does his interest seem to expand when more options are presented? As with previous studies that examine the optimal levels of complexity, what is the threshold after which exploration declines?31 Similarly, what is the relationship between physical exertion and curiosity? If the buttons are spaced farther apart, are they explored with the same intensity or frequency? Such measures would be valuable not only to other children with special needs but also to any educational or community setting serving a wide range of children and abilities.
Becca, Age 6
Becca is a six-and-a-half-year-old autistic girl, described by her mother as “happy,” a “free spirit,” and the “most confident person I know!” She dives into experiences without restraint and cares little for social etiquette. Becca will unabashedly arrive at school wearing costumes or outfits inspired by her favorite television characters, regularly choosing clothing based on its character-appeal and style over practicality. It is difficult to talk her out of an outfit with long sleeves and a sweater, even on a hot summer day, hinting that she can be equal parts creative and rigid in her actions and expressions.
Becca is a prolific artist, and she has reportedly never paused for even an instant before taking a marker to a piece of paper and beginning to draw. She can fill the pages of entire sketchbooks in rapid succession, never hesitating between one drawing and the next. She has no shortage of ideas, and always seems to know exactly what she wants. And while Becca’s confidence and sense of self are marked assets, she also does not feel motivated to do things that do not interest her, which can make academic work and other obligations difficult. For example, Becca will often spend recess exploring leaves and trees and making tiny piles of wood chips. Asking her to leave recess to do an activity of someone else’s choosing can prove challenging. Likewise, assessments and studies that require her to complete predetermined activities, including curiosity studies that examine her “spontaneous exploration” of a standardized selection of toys, are likely to be met with resistance and may elicit a performance not indicative of her true capabilities.32
Nevertheless, she exhibits undeniable exploratory behavior. Although she is not verbally curious like Mark, Becca will roam around her grandparents’ large yard, climbing rocks, looking at leaves, and bringing grass to her cheek to experience its texture and scent. She experiences the world in a decidedly physical way and can spend hours engaged in what appears to be a very compelling and reinforcing internal world. Some days she will sit and talk to herself (not always in words that others might understand), often laughing aloud at something she is thinking about.
This type of curiosity may be difficult to capture in a laboratory setting, and the few studies that have attempted to do so in the natural environments have relied on recorded narrations of behavior,33 which is time consuming to transcribe, quantify, and scale. Alternatively, multimodal data streams from video, audio, and wearable sensors enable rich new sources to objectively analyze naturalistic behavior and play.34 Combining off-the-shelf cameras and open-source data-processing tools, it is possible to track body pose,35 interactions, and gestures,36 as well as facial expressions37 and affect,38 without the use of specialized equipment or clinical settings. If tolerated, wearable eye-tracking glasses can also help monitor gaze and attention,39 differentiating the minute examination of a pile of wood chips from the inspection of ants marching home after a long day’s work. Machine learning and artificial intelligence enables statistical analysis of these records, including extensive datasets over days and weeks, allowing us to build computational models of behavior and curiosity. These models can help form a baseline of curiosity activities and emotions for individuals like Becca. We then can hypothesize how small changes might affect her curiosity in large ways, taking care to join her world instead of pigeonholing her into ours.
Although Becca can and does occasionally speak, her language remains limited, and approximately 90 percent of her conversations are “scripted” from favorite television characters and scenes. While some of this scripting is appropriate to the situation at hand, observers who are unfamiliar with her nuanced references may not understand the context and may interpret her communication as gibberish or “nonfunctional.” Abstract language remains difficult for her, and she does not ask or answer “why” questions. Yet she has entire television episodes memorized.
She is motivated by these television shows and seeks out opportunities to watch them, think about them, or draw pictures of them. As with many autistic affinities, the relationship she has with the shows and their characters can provide insight into her internal thought processes. For example, almost every picture she draws is a scene or a riff off a scene from an episode of a show. Likewise, if she repeats a line from a favorite episode and a parent or friend says the next line, her eyes will light up and she will smile gleefully as if to say, “Yes! You get it!” It is like a secret handshake, building trust and camaraderie, unlocking the passageway to her precious thoughts and feelings. The songs and plots from these shows can also help with transitions between activities, social situations, and difficult tasks, like acknowledging the need to go to the bathroom when in the middle of a favorite game. They seem to help organize her world, like a Rolodex of situations she can call upon to help process and relate to the emotions and experiences around her.
But these shows and characters may be more than just a conduit to her lush inner world. Becca’s nuanced relationship with them suggests that she is conceptualizing information and making connections far beyond what her words or general actions may indicate. She may be exploring whole theoretical worlds in her head that can only be perceived if one joins her world and explores with her.
So how could we build neurodiverse curiosity studies for Becca? How is her curiosity expressed in the kingdom of her characters? If her characters ask “why” or “I wonder” questions, how does that influence the wonder she expresses through any modality—actions, scripting, or spontaneous speech? What if puppets of her characters explore her real world with inquisitive attitudes and actions? Does she join in? Does she imitate them or learn from them? How do small changes to the ways these characters explore affect how Becca incorporates their words or behaviors into her own life? Even with her favorite shows, she tends to relate more strongly to one character than the rest—for example, Prince Wednesday from Daniel Tiger or Gekko from PJ Masks. How does her assimilation of the information change when Prince Wednesday demonstrates curiosity behaviors versus the default protagonist Daniel Tiger? What is the effect of using words or songs versus only actions? These results may generalize to broader curiosity studies, especially in the context of curiosity and technology,40 but until we understand the most fundamental drivers of these exceptional individuals, we will never know.
This chapter proposed a strengths-based research framework for neurodiverse curiosity studies. Through three distinct case studies, we explored how individual manifestations of curiosity may depend on interests, environment, and skills, and we noted how typical approaches to the study of curiosity may fail for autistic and neurodiverse children. We then described how novel methods and engineering solutions could personalize the approach to elicit and measure curiosity and examined how small parametric changes could significantly affect exploratory behavior outcomes. With these examples and techniques, we invite researchers to form and test new hypotheses, probing how curiosity evolves over time, how it is motivated by personal interests, and how it can be expressed both with and without language. We look eagerly to future studies that expand our understanding of curiosity in all individuals.
Notes
This work was supported by the MIT Media Lab Consortium and Learning Initiative. Special thanks to Perry Zurn and Arjun Shankar for inspiration and thoughtful feedback, and to the individuals and families represented by the case studies.
George Loewenstein, “The Psychology of Curiosity: A Review and Reinterpretation,” Psychological Bulletin 116, no. 1 (1994): 75.
William James, Talks to Teachers on Psychology and to Students on Some of Life’s Ideals, vol. 12 (1899; Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1983).
Jean Piaget and Margaret Cook, The Origins of Intelligence in Children, vol. 8 (New York: International Universities Press, 1952).
Daniel E. Berlyne, “A Theory of Human Curiosity,” British Journal of Psychology 45, no. 3 (1954): 180–91; Daniel E. Berlyne, Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960).
Loewenstein, “The Psychology of Curiosity.”
William R. Charlesworth, “Instigation and Maintenance of Curiosity Behavior as a Function of Surprise versus Novel and Familiar Stimuli,” Child Development (1964): 1169–86.
Harry F. Harlow, Margaret K. Harlow, and Donald R. Meyer, “Learning Motivated by a Manipulation Drive,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 40, no. 2 (1950): 228.
Celeste Kidd and Benjamin Y. Hayden, “The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity,” Neuron 88, no. 3 (2015): 449–60.
Susan Harter and Edward Zigler, “The Assessment of Effectance Motivation in Normal and Retarded Children,” Developmental Psychology 10, no. 2 (1974): 169.
Corinne Hutt, “Exploration, Arousal and Autism,” Psychologische Forschung 33, no. 1 (1969): 1–8.
Corinne Hutt, “Temporal Effects on Response Decrement and Stimulus Satiation in Exploration,” British Journal of Psychology 58, nos. 3–4 (1967): 365–73.
Leo Kanner, “Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact,” Nervous Child 2, no. 3 (1943): 217–50.
Daniel E. Berlyne, “Curiosity and Exploration,” Science 153, no. 3731 (1966): 25–33.
Noah J. Sasson et al., “Children with Autism Demonstrate Circumscribed Attention during Passive Viewing of Complex Social and Nonsocial Picture Arrays,” Autism Research 1, no. 1 (2008): 31–42.
Center for Disease Control, Communicating with and about People with Disabilities, https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/disabilityandhealth/pdf/disabilityposter_photos.pdf.
Lydia Brown, “Identity-First Language,” ASAN, 2011, http://autisticadvocacy.org/about-asan/identity-first-language; Doman Lum, Culturally Competent Practice: A Framework for Understanding, 4th ed. (Belmont, Calif.: Cengage Learning, 2010).
Steven Pinker, “The Game of the Name,” New York Times, April 3, 1994.
David W. Hung, “Generalization of ‘Curiosity’ Questioning Behavior in Autistic Children,” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 8, no. 3 (1977): 237–45; Frederick F. Schmitt and Reza Lahroodi, “The Epistemic Value of Curiosity,” Educational Theory 58, no. 2 (2008): 125–48.
Schulamith Kreitler, Edward Zigler, and Hans Kreitler, “The Nature of Curiosity in Children,” Journal of School Psychology 13, no. 3 (1975): 185–200.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed. (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013).
Ron Suskind, Life, Animated: A Story of Sidekicks, Heroes, and Autism (New York: Kingswell, 2014).
Kristina T. Johnson and Rosalind W. Picard, “SPRING: Customizable, Motivation-Driven Technology for Children with Autism or Neurodevelopmental Differences,” in Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Interaction Design and Children (2017), 149–58.
Richard Arend, Frederick L. Gove, and L. Alan Sroufe, “Continuity of Individual Adaptation from Infancy to Kindergarten: A Predictive Study of Ego-Resiliency and Curiosity in Preschoolers,” Child Development 50, no. 4 (1979): 950–59.
Richard M. Ryan and Edward L. Deci, “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions,” Contemporary Educational Psychology 25, no 1 (2000): 54–67.
Berlyne, “A Theory of Human Curiosity.”
Berlyne, Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity.
Jaime Jirout and David Klahr, “Children’s Scientific Curiosity: In Search of an Operational Definition of an Elusive Concept,” Developmental Review 32, no. 2 (2012): 125–60.
Ming-Zher Poh, Nicholas C. Swenson, and Rosalind W. Picard, “A Wearable Sensor for Unobtrusive, Long-Term Assessment of Electrodermal Activity,” IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 57, no. 5 (2010): 1243–52.
Subhas Chandra Mukhopadhyay, “Wearable Sensors for Human Activity Monitoring: A Review,” IEEE Sensors 15, no. 3 (2015): 1321–30; Javier Hernandez et al., “Bioglass: Physiological Parameter Estimation Using a Head-Mounted Wearable Device,” in The 4th International Conference on Wireless Mobile Communication and Healthcare (Mobihealth) (2014), 55–58.
Ming-Zher Poh, Daniel J. McDuff, and Rosalind W. Picard, “Non-Contact, Automated Cardiac Pulse Measurements Using Video Imaging and Blind Source Separation,” Optics Express 18, no. 10 (2010): 10762–74.
Harvey N. Switzky, H. Carl Haywood, and Robert Isett, “Exploration, Curiosity, and Play in Young Children: Effects of Stimulus Complexity,” Developmental Psychology 10, no. 3 (1974): 321.
Paul McReynolds, Mary Acker, and Caryl Pietila, “Relation of Object Curiosity to Psychological Adjustment in Children,” Child Development 32 (1961): 393–400.
Patricia Minuchin, “Correlates of Curiosity and Exploratory Behavior in Preschool Disadvantaged Children,” Child Development 42 (1971): 939–50.
James M. Rehg et al., “Behavioral Imaging and Autism,” IEEE Pervasive Computing 2 (2014): 84–87.
Zhe Cao et al., “Realtime Multi-Person 2D Pose Estimation Using Part Affinity Fields,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR) (2017), 7291–99.
Siddharth S. Rautaray and Anupam Agrawal, “Vision Based Hand Gesture Recognition for Human Computer Interaction: A Survey,” Artificial Intelligence Review 43, no. 1 (2015): 1–54.
Tadas Baltrusaitis, Peter Robinson, and Louis-Philippe Morency, “OpenFace: An Open Source Facial Behavior Analysis Toolkit,” in IEEE Winter Conference on Applications of Computer Vision (WACV) (2016), 1–10.
Daniel McDuff et al., “AFFDEX SDK: A Cross-Platform Real-Time Multi-Face Expression Recognition Toolkit,” in Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (2016), 3723–26.
Jacqueline Gottlieb et al., “Information-Seeking, Curiosity, and Attention: Computational and Neural Mechanisms,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, no. 11 (2013): 585–93.
Marilyn P. Arnone et al., “Curiosity, Interest and Engagement in Technology-Pervasive Learning Environments: A New Research Agenda,” Educational Technology Research and Development 59, no. 2 (2011): 181–98.
We use cookies to analyze our traffic. Please decide if you are willing to accept cookies from our website. You can change this setting anytime in Privacy Settings.